Decision Analysis in Healthcare
 

 

Modeling Group Decisions


 

Introduction

There are many occasions in which a group, rather than an individual, has to make a decision.  A good example is the board of director's decision about a major capital purchase.  In these circumstances, it maybe important to model the group's decision so that it can be easily communicated to others or so that the group can have more self insight.   This chapter discusses how an analyst can model a group's decision.

A model of a group of decision makers could be thought of as an average of the models of various members of the group.  If so, a model is built for each individual member and the parameters of the models are averaged to represent the group.  But groups are not just a collection of individuals.  There is a synergy of thought and ideas that emerges from successful groups.  Mathematical aggregation of individual's models into a group model loses the real advantage of having the group -- the real reason for having a group in the first place: to arrive better decisions than the best member of the group.  To effectively model a group's decision, one should create a model that reflects the group's consensus, which maybe very different than the average of each individual's input.  

Decision analysts have for sometime focused on mathematical methods of aggregating the judgment of various experts or decision makers (Jacobs 1995, Meyer, Booker 1991).  This chapter does not review the literature on mathematical aggregation of experts' opinions.  Instead, this chapter focuses on behavioral methods of getting a group to come to a consensus around a model.    The goal is to design a process, where group members can, after reasonable deliberation, agree around a specific model and its parameters.

The task of structuring a decision and estimating the parameters of the model requires a great deal of input from the group.  The model building effort may be seen as an artificial process to most decision makers.  The questions posed by the analyst may seem a contorted way of looking at the decision at hand.  The analyst's frequent interruption to structure and assess various estimates may interfere with the group's deliberations.  If a group's decision has to be modeled effectively, then a process needs to be found that constructs a model without interfering with the group's interactions.  This chapter provides one such group process.   

The chapter starts with a short history of commonly used approaches to structured group meetings.  Research on group processes started in earnest in late sixties and early seventies.  It has grown in recent years to studies of group decision support systems, where computers are used to facilitate meetings.  Our review is selective as the literature is vast.  After the short history, we present a new approach called Integrative Group Process (IGP). This approach borrows from many of the existing methods of improving group processes. 

A Short History of Group  Processes

To understand what works in group processes, we start with one of the simplest group processes, the Group Communication Strategy.  This approach originates from a set of normative instructions proposed by Hall and Watson (1970). Before the meeting starts, members are instructed to:

  • Avoid arguing,
  • Avoid win-lose statements,
  • Avoid changing opinions in order to reduce conflict,
  • Avoid conflict-reducing techniques such as majority votes and bargaining.
  • View differences of opinion as natural and initial agreements as suspect.

No other process change is made.  Group members talk as they wish about  topics they choose, with no break in how and when various components of the meeting are accomplished.  During these conversations, the analyst constructs a model of the group, with each member contributing at will and  commenting on what is relevant.  After the start of the meeting, group members may or may not follow the recommended rules for interaction.  The expectation is that most will follow these simple instructions, if reminded to do so at start of the meeting.  In the decade following Watson and Hall's  work, a number of studies evaluated their recommendations.  Early research showed that the Group Communication Strategy leads to high quality judgments without sacrificing the group's later acceptance of the judgments (Nemiroff,  Pasmore, and Ford 1976). But the study was done with student subjects, in situations where status differences may not exist.  When there are large status differences, group members weigh the opinions and suggestions of high status persons more heavily (Forsyth 1998, Pagliari, Grimshaw 2002).  In these groups, the Group Communication Strategy may not do well.  Furthermore, little is known about the success of normative instructions in situations where group members are in conflict or have substantial stakes in the final group judgment.  A process was needed that could work in these circumstances. 

Another approach to helping groups talk through their differences is the Nominal Group Process (Delbecq, Van de Ven and Gustafson, 1975).  This is a generic name for face-to-face group techniques in which instructions are given to group members not to interact with each other except at specific steps in the process.  The following are the steps in the process: 

  1. Silent idea generations
  2. Round-robin sharing of ideas
  3. Feedback to the group
  4. Explanatory group discussion
  5. Individual re-assessment, and
  6. Mathematical aggregation of revised judgments

The Nominal Group Process produces a prioritized list of ideas as well as  numerical estimates in a short time frame.   For example, an analyst can use this process to solicit attributes for a value model and then repeat the process to solicit weights for the attributes.  This approach remains popular despite the fact that it has been more than 30 years since its inception (Moon 1999, Carney,  McIntosh, Worth 1996) and is one of the key approaches used to develop treatment algorithms and consensus panels ((Cruse, Winiarek, Marshburn, Clark, Djulbegovic 2002).   Research on the Nominal Group Process is extensive.  A recent review  (Black, Murphy, Lamping et al. 1999) shows that in numerous circumstances the process produces better results than unstructured group interactions.  The performance of Nominal Group Process may depend on task structure, the selection of  participants, the presentation of the scientific information available to the group, the way group interaction is structured, and the method of synthesizing individual  judgments.  Given the various factors that affect the performance of Nominal Group Process, analysts should rely on what makes sense.  In our view, three components of the Nominal group process explain when it will be successful in managing groups:

  • First, ideas should not be evaluated one at a time. Rather the analyst should collect many ideas before any one of them are evaluated. Postponing evaluation increases creative solutions.
  • Second, in estimating numbers, thinking again improves the accuracy of the numbers. A sort of bootstrapping occurs, where the group members best themselves by listening to other group members and revising their own opinions.
  • Finally, individual generation of ideas leads to more ideas and more creative ones than generating ideas while listening to other group members.

Despite widespread use of Nominal Group Process in consensus panels (Jones and Hunter, 1995), the process is not without serious problems.  In tasks that  require judging the worth of several alternatives, e.g. in developing a  multi-attribute value model, this technique may produce judgments inferior to  the judgment of the most knowledgeable group member (Holloman and Hendrick 1972;  Nemiroff, Pasmore and Ford 1976). But by far, the most serious problem with the  process is that participants feel awkward about restrictions in their  interactions. After the group meeting they may feel that the process and not  they led them to the conclusions they arrived at. Therefore, they may not be  committed to the group's consensus.  A group of experts reviewing the data on group processes recommended that when acceptance of the group's decision is crucial as to whether the model is put to use, less structured group processes produce more widely accepted group decisions (Rotondi, 1999). 

Another technique widely used by consensus groups in health care settings is the Delphi process  (Jones and Hunter 1995).  Dalkey and Helmer (1963) designed this process as a non face-to-face procedure for aggregating group members' opinions. Group members answer three interrelated questionnaires, the analyst summarizes the responses from each survey and mails the synthesis back to the same or other groups  (Ryan, Scott et al. 2001) for further  comment.  For example, the first questionnaire may ask the group members to describe the attributes in a value model, the second questionnaire may present  the attributes and ask for revisions as well as weighting of the relative  importance of the attributes and the third questionnaire may continue with the  revisions of the model as well as present a set of scenarios and ask the  respondents to rate them.  In some applications, for example in forecasting  technological changes based on insights of a large group of experts, the Delphi  method has proven useful . Delphi is also useful in situations where conflict or status  differences among group members are so strong as to be dysfunctional. The most  insightful feature of the Delphi process is that a meeting of minds can occur without an actual meeting.

Studies show that face-to-face interaction is superior to Delphi's remote and private opinion gathering (Cho, Turoff, Hiltz,  2003, Woudenberg 1991). Two studies comparing Nominal Group Process to Delphi found the two techniques about equal (Seaver, 1977; Miner, 1976).  In  other studies, Delphi has been less accurate than Nominal Group Process. In  one study, for example, Delphi's remote feedback was detrimental and reduced the  accuracy of the group members' estimates (Gustafson et al. 1973).

Difficulty with existing group processes has led a number of investigators to design their own modeling process that can be used in group settings.   These approaches often use computers to help model the decision maker in a group setting.  In the 1990s, a large number of studies were published regarding  what affects the performance of group decision support systems (Fjermestad and  Hiltz 1997).  The Social Judgment Analysis (Hammond et al., 1976) is a  group process that uses computers to model each member as they actively interact  with each other.  It was designed to reduce the pressure for group members to comply with the group's mind set just to feel more accepted by the group. The following are the steps in the process:

  1. Group members meet in face to face meetings, in which they have access to a computer.
  2. Each group member rates a series of scenarios. A scenario is a combination of clues that affect a judgment. For example in judging credit worthiness of companies, the clues may be last year's profit, changes in market share, and management changes. A scenario is constructed by varying levels of these three clues. In the first scenario the company was not profitable last year but has gained a significant market share and its management has been stable. In the second scenario, the company may be profitable but have a small market share and a stable management. More scenarios can be constructed by differing the levels of each clue.
  3. The computer analyzes the group members ratings to see which factors affect the judgment most. This is usually done through regressing the scenario ratings on the elements of or the clues in the scenarios.
  4. The computer informs the group member concerning the results of the analysis: it tells them that the way they rated the scenarios suggests that certain clues are most important in their judgment. It then lists these clues.
  5. Group members often do not agree with the results of the analysis and revise their ratings of the scenarios so that the ratings best fit with what they consider most important.
  6. Once group members come to terms with the way they wish to judge the scenarios, a consensus model is developed and used to represent the group's judgment.

Computer facilitated meetings, in general, and Social Judgment Analysis, in particular, may seem too much work for some meetings.  But with growing use of  computers, many meetings are occurring through computers anyway.  Many decision makers are in different locations and must use the computer to collaborate.  Naturally, in these settings Group Decision Support Systems can help improve the self insight of individual group members and eventually the quality of and speed of arriving at group consensus.  The value of this technique is demonstrated in recent studies focused on helping clinicians understand their own judgments (Holzworth  and Wills. 1999).  Social Judgment Analysis works well because it focuses decision maker's attention on why they prefer an alternative as opposed to which alternative they  prefer.  It provides group feedback that helps decision makers focus their  reasoning.  Data show that people change their opinion to conform with  group norms. While this behavior is healthy for keeping the peace in the group, it is counter productive if ideas are being judged based on the popularity as  opposed to their merits. Rohrbaugh's 1979 study showed that when feedback  focused on why an idea was preferred as opposed to which idea was more popular,  the group's final judgment was more accurate.  Rohrbaugh also showed that Social Judgment Analysis was more accurate than the Delphi method as well as the Nominal Group Process (Rohrbaugh  1981) .

Interest in use of computers in group processes has led to many innovations.   Eden, Jone and Sims (1983) were one of the first to develop a process for modeling how a group arrives at its judgments through "Cognitive Mapping."  This  process starts with constructing two parallel statements of the problem. One  showing the factors leading to the problem, the other showing the factors  leading to a satisfactory solution. For example, the problem may be stated as  "high labor costs," the solution may be stated as "lowering the labor cost." The causes of high labor cost and the factors leading to lowering labor costs are  also organized.  For example, a cause of the problem may be "shortage of  qualified worker."  A solution may be "more availability of qualified workers."  Clearly, causes of the problem and factors leading to the solutions are related,  usually a change of adjectives produces the other. Through linguistic manipulation of the statement of the problem, Cognitive Mapping hopes to  stimulate new ideas.

Eden and colleagues, when they use Cognitive Mapping, often collect the group member's ideas about the problem and its solutions separately and then revise  these ideas in a face-to-face meeting. Occasionally they quantify the influence  of causes and effects through a Round Robin process, where group members write  down their estimates and share them afterwards. They, then, simulate how  changing one factor may affect the problem. These simulations may lead to new  insights into the problem.  Cognitive Mapping of groups of decision makers continues to be an active area of research (Vennix 2000).

Social Judgment Analysis and Cognitive Mapping Group Process were the start of many innovative methods of computer facilitated collaboration.  McLeod (1992) summarized the group decision support  literature and found that computer-facilitated group meetings increased decision quality, led to more equal participation by group members and more focus on task and less focus on social networking and support.  At the same time, McLeod identified that computer facilitated group interaction decreased consensus and member satisfaction with the group meeting.  The reduction in member satisfaction with group process might be due to the central role of a facilitator in controlling who speaks when in group processes (Austin, Liker, McLeod 1993).     Strauss (1997) showed that computer facilitated groups had lower cohesiveness and group satisfaction than face to face groups, primarily because of the rate with which the group members interacted.  Even a simple technology such as teleconferencing has been shown to have detrimental effects on group discussion and processes (Alemi, et al. 1997).  Clearly face to face meetings, when it is possible and when it is run well, is more efficient in getting ideas across to group members and as a consequence of this improved efficiency, group members feel that others have heard their point of view.  

Summary of Lessons Learned & Principles Recommended

 

What has been learned?

There are many group processes that can be used to model a group's decision. A review of some of these processes creates bewildering methods of  conducting team work. This section reviews key lessons learned from research on effective team processes. Three and half decades of research on effective group work point to following lessons:

  • Postpone evaluation. It is best to separate idea generation from idea evaluations. When evaluation is postponed, more ideas and more creative ideas emerge.
  • Think again. It is best to think through the decision again, especially when numerical estimates are involved. In repetition, people gain confidence in what they are doing and can see pitfalls previously missed.
  • Meet before the meeting. It is useful to get input from group members individually, before they can influence each other.  This can often occur through use of computers and might be one way of combining computer facilitated decision support with face to face meetings.
  • Judge the merit of ideas. It is important to evaluate ideas based on their merit and not based on their popularity.  Successful group processes separate ideas from the originator of the idea.  In this fashion, ideas are judged based on their merits and not who proposed them.
  • Instruct the group to behave. It is best to instruct group members to keep calm and accept conflict as productive.  Simple instructions at start of meeting can set the tone of group discussion to come.
  • Use computer to facilitate components of the meeting.  It is best to use technology to help the groups arrive at consensus but such use should not reduce the rate of exchange of ideas or the ease with which members interact with each other.

The Integrative Group Process (IGP) is based on the lessons learned in the last 30 years of research on group processes:

  • Like Nominal Group Process, IGP postpone evaluation of ideas until the analyst has collected the ideas of all members.  In addition, both approaches improve estimates of relative importance of ideas through repetition (i.e. both require the group member to assess, discuss, and revise their numerical estimates). 
  • Like Delphi, IGP obtains remote and private opinions. But unlike Delphi, these remote contributions are followed by face-to-face interactions.
  • Like the Group Communication instructions, IGP sets ground rules for free-form group interaction.
  • Like Cognitive Mapping and the Social Judgment Analysis, IGP focuses discussion on the group member's reasoning rather than the group's decision.  Thus group members can better understand why they disagree on a point if they come to see each other's reasoning.
  • Like computer facilitated group processes, IGP collects ideas from group members through computers: typically by emails.  IGP and computer facilitated group process differ in what occurs after the initial collection of ideas.  IGP emphasizes face to face meeting after computer facilitated collection of ideas. 

Integrative Group Process

The process we recommend for your use in modeling a group's decision is called Integrative Group Process.   It is an eclectic group process based on more than 30 years of research on teamwork and group interaction literature.  This process has 6 steps:

  1. Select the best experts, despite their conflicts
  2. Meet before the meeting to make a "straw model"
  3. Redo the model during the meeting
  4. Estimate the parameters of the model
  5. Discuss major differences and re-estimate the parameters
  6. Ignore small differences & prepare report

Step 1: Select the Best

 

What is the idea?

The composition of the group is an important and generally controllable aspect of the group.  Occasionally, analyst may avoid putting too many high status members in the group -- fearing that they would not be able to work together.  This is a mistake.  The best experts and decision makers must be invited to participate in the meeting, without them crucial information will be missing from the meeting and quality of the decision may be affected.  Instead of avoiding conflict, the IGP process helps manage conflict among group member so that productive quality work can be accomplished despite status differences and presence of a history of conflict. 

The analyst needs to think about what portion of the group should come from inside versus outside of the organization.  If employees closest to the process are invited to the decision making group, then the group's decisions are more likely to be implemented.  If people removed from the process, perhaps experts outside the organization, are engaged, then more radical solutions may be proposed  (Stumpf, 1978).  In the end a balance needs to be struck between the percent of the group selected from inside or outside of the organization.   

The size of  the group should depend on its purpose. Experiments with groups of various size have shown that if the quality of the group's solution is of considerable importance, it is useful to include a large number of members (e.g., seven to nine) so that many inputs are available to the group in making its decision.  If the degree of consensus is of primary importance, it is useful to choose a smaller group (e.g., five to seven) so that members can have their opinions considered and discussed (Cummings,  Huber, and Arendt, 1974, and Manners, 1975).  It is a general rule of thumb that  the group size should not be smaller than three to five. Groups that meet face to face should not have more than nine members as each member may not be able to participate adequately.

Heterogeneity of the group's background is closely related to the size of the  group and is another important aspect of design of successful groups.  A necessary, though not sufficient, requirement for accurate group judgments is to  have an appropriate knowledge pool in the group. Since no one person is an  expert in all aspects of a problem, diverse backgrounds and expertise are  imperative for achieving this heterogeneity. Involving people from different  functional units of the organizations helps bring different expertise to the  problem. Difference in background and knowledge could, however, accentuate the  conflict between the group members and, if neither originality nor quality are  criteria for evaluating the team's work, the analyst should select group  members to minimize differences in their backgrounds.

Getting people to devote their time to a meeting is difficult.  Many remember wasted efforts in other meetings and avoid new meetings. The analyst can take several steps to increase participation. First, examine the  purpose of the meeting.  If it is difficult to obtain participation, perhaps the the problem being addressed is not important. People who are close to a problem, invariably care about it and are willing to address the problem.  But if they  feel the problem is not real, or the search for solution is a formality, they are less likely to participate.

Second, the analyst can improve meeting participation by clear communications concerning the meeting logistics and expectations. The communication should clarify why is the meeting important, why is it useful to model the task, and what can be expected at the end of the meeting. It should clarify the logistics of the meeting (i.e., when, where and how) and emphasize that the meeting is an ad hoc group.

Third, it maybe useful to remind the invited group members about who else is being invited. People are more likely to come to meetings where people they admire are present.  It also helps to emphasize who nominated the potential group member and that there are very few people asked to participate. It should be clear that the group member's contribution is unique and valued. Finally, it helps if group members are reimbursed for their time by providing them with an honorarium.

Step 2: Meet Before the Meeting

 

No-surprises

 

What is the idea?

Before face-to-face meeting, group members are individually interviewed and modeled.  If group members live far apart the interviews are done by phone, a series of emails, or through computer connections. Whether done remotely or face-to-face, the interview is scheduled ahead of time.  During the interview, which takes roughly one hour, the analyst explains the group's task, elicits the participant's model (attributes or clues), asks the participant to estimate utilities or probabilities, and walks the participant through the steps in the IGP process, so there will be no  surprises in the actual meeting.  The bulk of the interview time, however, is  spent listening to the group member and trying to model the reasoning behind his or her choices. 

For example, if the group is to create a decision tree, the analyst asks the member to do so and listens to why specific steps are important in the decision. If the group's task is to suggest alternative solutions, the analyst obtains a list of viable alternatives and tries to understand what  evaluation criteria are important for evaluating these alternative.  If the group member's opinions depend on many rules, the analyst solicits these rules and makes sure that the mathematical model reflects them. The point is that no matter what the task is, the analyst tries to not only accomplish the task but also understand the reasoning behind it.

It is important to keep in mind while the bulk of interview time and interactions are spent on structuring the decision, the analyst must also assess utilities and probabilities so that the participant understands what is going to happen at the meeting.  A brief training in probability concepts might also be useful before assessing probabilities. 

After interviewing each group member individually, the analyst collates the responses of all participants and creates a "straw model" of the decision. A straw model refers to a decision model designed to be redone.  The face-to-face meeting starts with a presentation of the  "straw model" and proceeds with a request for improvements. Constructing the model before the group meeting has three advantages:

  1. It increases the accuracy of the group's judgment by ensuring that the group process will not prevent an individual from presenting his/her ideas.
  2. The interviews prior to the meeting save the group some working time by collecting members' ideas before the meeting.
  3. Because the "straw model" serves as a blue print to guide the group discussion, it saves additional time by partitioning the group task into smaller, more manageable discussions.

Step 3:  Redo the Straw Model

 

Better the Next Time Around

 

What is the idea?

The analyst lists the basic structure of the "straw model" (the attributes used in evaluation task, the decision events used in a tree structure, or the clues used in a forecast) on flip charts; one component per chart. The flip charts are spread around the room so any member can see what has been said to date. The group convenes to revise the "straw model".

The analyst introduces himself/herself, explains the group's task and  agenda, restates the importance of the task, and asks members to introduce  themselves.  These introductions as an important part of the  group process.  If members are not explicitly introduced, they will do so implicitly through out their deliberations. 

The analyst presents the "straw model," asks the group to revise the model, and focuses group's attention on one of the components in the "straw model"  as a starting point. The focus on one component at a time is an important  way of managing group's time and conflicts.  As group members suggest new  ideas or modifications, the analyst records them on the appropriate pages in front of the group.  Thus, the analyst serves as a secretary to the group making sure that ideas are not lost. Recording the comments reassures the group members that their ideas are being put in front of the group for further  consideration. The process continues until the group identifies, discusses and  revises all relevant components in the decision structure.

Through active listening (e.g., nodding, asking for clarification) and  recording of group member ideas on the flip charts, the analyst plays the important role of directing the discussion, preventing premature closure of  ideas (Van de Ven and Delbecq, 1971), returning the group to task related activities, distributing the group's time over different aspects of the task,  restraining expression of critical comments during the generation of ideas, and always separating people from ideas so that ideas are judged on their own merits. The analyst uses the instructions developed for Group Communication Strategy as guidelines  for this phase of interaction. The analyst should not participate in the content of the discussions; and should not reword what has been said in his/her own terms.

Step 4:  Estimate Model Parameters

 

Let Numbers Do the Talking

 

What is the idea?

In this step of IGP, the analyst helps the group put numbers to the decision components (e.g. assess weights for the utility models, assess likelihood ratios associated with various clues). This task is done individually and without discussion until a major difference between the group members is identified.   While working individually, the group remains in the presence  of one another. Seeing each other working helps the group members exert more effort on the task at hand. As the group proceeds, the analyst collects the group's responses and puts the answers on a flip chart. The scores are not listed  in the flip chart in a manner that identifies who has said it.  One purpose of this step is to encourage the group to consider the merit of ideas and not  who has expressed them.  When there are major differences among the estimates, the analyst asks the group (not any particular person) to stop working individually and explain their reasoning to the group.

Step 5: Discuss Major Differences

 

Discus Issues not Each Other

 

What is the idea?

IGP focuses the group's discussion on the group's logic. Instead of  discussing differences in numerical ratings, the group discusses the reasons behind their ratings. This approach to discussion has been shown to reduce conflict among group members (Hammond et al., 1976; Rohrbaugh, 1979).  Disagreements among group members have many different sources. Some disagreements are due to fatigue and unclear problem specification. These disagreements are reduced through clarifying the assumptions behind group member's perspectives. Still other disagreements are due to differences in  knowledge and experience.  Discussion may reduce these conflicts if group member's succeed in communicating the rationale for their judgments.  Some group members may raise considerations others have missed.  Group members may see flaws in the logic. Other disagreements are due to value differences.  Better communication may not reduce this type of conflict. IGP reduces conflicts that are due to  misconceptions and miscommunications and accepts other conflicts as inherent in the task.

In order to save group time, the analyst should identify major differences among members, and focus the group's attention on them. Small differences are probably due to errors in estimating numbers and not due to substantive issues. When there are major differences in numerical ratings, the analyst stops the group members from working individually and asks for a discussion. This is done without identifying what is the group average (norm) or  who are the people whose ratings differed from the average.  Disclosures of  the members involved could have ill effects by polarizing the group. For example, Castore and Muringham (1978) showed that disclosure of individual  member preferences lowers the later support for the group's decision. The person who disagrees is not as important as the existence of the disagreement and the need to resolve the disagreement through discussion.

After the group has discussed their differences, the analyst asks group members to individually re-estimate the model parameters. This process of estimating,  discussing, and re-estimating leads to more accurate results than the use of other processes that eliminate anyone of the three steps (Gustafson et al., 1973).


Step 6: Ignore Small Differences

Show Consensus


What is the idea?

Although the analyst encourages group members to resolve their differences through discussion, at some point, it is necessary to stop the  interaction and mathematically resolve minor group differences, such as by averaging the estimates from various group members. If major differences remain,  it is necessary to report these differences.  In a few days after the meeting has ended: a report is written about the meeting. This report contains several different topics including the following:

  • Why was the meeting convened?
  • Methods used to facilitate the meeting and analyze the findings.
  • A detailed description of the consensus model structure
  • Did group participants arrive at a consensus regarding model parameters (extent of agreement in final numerical ratings of various group members)
  • The average parameters assessed by the group.
  • Conclusions and next steps in the group's task.

A document about the group deliberation is important not only to people who were in the meeting but also to people who were not. Cinokur and Burnstein  (1974) had individual subjects list the persuasiveness of pro and con arguments.  The net balance of persuasiveness of the arguments correlated with attitude change produced by group discussion.  But, more important, other individuals not  present in the group discussion, who were exposed to the same arguments, changed  their attitudes in the same way. The work of Cinokur and Burnstein shows that  the information content of group discussion is important in convincing people outside the group.  A well documented decision model can help convince others.

What Do You Know?

Advanced learners like you, often need different ways of understanding a  topic. Reading is just one way of understanding. Another way is through writing.  When you write you not only recall what you have written but also may need to  make inferences about what you have read.

  1. List the steps in the nominal group process?
  2. How is Integrative Group Process different from Nominal Group Process?
  3. Of all the techniques you have learned here, which process or component of the process you can use in your project groups. Be specific for why are you preferring one approach to another.
  4. In what group processes is there a delay in evaluation of ideas.  Why would postponing evaluation help the group process?
  5. If experts have conflicts among them or employees have sharp status difference, should we avoid having them in the same group?
  6. What is the point of asking for utility or probability assessment before the group meets?
  7. Which is better, to change the group meeting process or to instruct group members to stay on task and avoid falling into conflicts?
  8. In what steps within the Integrative Group Process, group members work with numbers?  In what steps in the Integrative Group Process, group members specify the structure of the decision model?
  9. What is a straw model?

Send your response by email to your instructor.  Include both the question and the answers in your response.  Include your contact information.

Slides and Narrated Slides

To assist you in reviewing the material in this lecture, please see slides.  In addition, you can listen to the following three-part lecture.  Part one reviews the literature:

 

Part two continues with the review.  See part two of the lecture:

 

Part three of the lecture focuses on the Integrative Group Process:

 

See also slides on empowerment and cooperation.   

More

  1. Recent research on team effectiveness.
  2. Creating cohesion through changes in group processes. 
  3. Mosel and Shamp discus methods for enhancing quality improvement team effectiveness. 
  4. Impact of teamwork on outcomes of care.  
  5. Enhancing quality improvement teams.
  6. Bibliography on cost effectiveness of clinical teams of nurses and physicians.
  7. Assessing a teams potential problem solving skills.

References

  1. Alemi F , Jackson M, Parren T, Williams L, Cavor B, Llorens S, Mosavel M.  Participation in Teleconference Support Groups: Application to Drug-Using Pregnant Patients.  Journal of Medical Systems 1997,  Vol 21, 2:  pages: 119 - 125.
  2. Austin LC, Liker JK, McLeod PL.  Who Controls the Technology in Group Support Systems? Determinants and Consequences  Human-Computer Interaction, Vol. 8, No. 3: pages 217-236. 1993.
  3. Black N, Murphy M, Lamping D,  McKee M, Sanderson C, Askham J, Marteau T.  Consensus development  methods: a review of best practice in creating clinical guidelines. J Health Serv Res Policy. 1999 Oct;4(4):236-48.
  4. Carney O, McIntosh J, Worth A.   The use of the Nominal Group Technique in research with community  nurses. J Adv Nurs. 1996 May;23(5):1024-9.
  5. Cho HK, Turoff M, Hiltz SR.   The Impacts of Delphi Communication Structure on Small and Medium Sized  Asynchronous Groups.  HICSS, 2003
  6. Cruse H, Winiarek M, Marshburn J, Clark O, Djulbegovic B. Quality and methods of developing practice guidelines. BMC Health Serv Res. 2002; 2
  7. Fjermestad J, Hiltz SR. "An  Analysis of the Effects of Mode of Communication on Group Decision  Making," HICSS, vol. 01, no. 1, p. 17, January 1998.
  8. Forsyth DR. Group Dynamics, Wadsworth Publishing; 3rd edition, 1998.
  9. Holzworth RJ, Wills CE. Nurses'  judgments regarding seclusion and restraint of psychiatric patients: a  social judgment analysis. Res Nurs Health. 1999 Jun;22(3):189-201.
  10. Jacobs RA. Methods for combining experts' probability assessments. Neural Comput. 1995 Sep; 7(5): 867-88.
  11. Jones J, Hunter D. Consensus  methods for medical and health services research. BMJ. 1995 Aug  5;311(7001):376-80.
  12. McLeod PL.  University of Michigan An Assessment of the Experimental Literature on Electronic Support of Group Work: Results of a Meta-Analysis.  Computer Interaction 1992, Vol. 7, No. 3, Pages 257-280. 
  13. Meyer, M. and J. Booker. 1991. Eliciting and Analyzing Expert Judgment: A Practical Guide. Knowledge-Based Systems, Vol. 5 Academic Press, London.
  14. Moon RH. Finding diamonds in the  trenches with the nominal group process. Fam Pract Manag. 1999  May;6(5):49-50.
  15. Rohrbaugh J. Improving the Quality  of Group Judgment: Social Judgment Analysis and the Nominal Group  Technique. Organiz. Behav. And Human Perform. Vol. 28, no. 2, pp.  272-288. 1981
  16. Pagliari C, Grimshaw J.  Impact of group structure and process on multidisciplinary evidence-based guideline development: an observational study. J Eval Clin Pract. 2002 May;8(2):145-53.
  17. Rotondi AJ. Assessing a team's problem solving ability: evaluation of the Team Problem Solving Assessment Tool (TPSAT). Health Care Manag Sci. 1999 Dec;2(4):205-14.
  18. Ryan M, Scott DA, Reeves C, Bate  A, van Teijlingen ER, Russell EM, Napper M, Robb CM. Eliciting public  preferences for healthcare: a systematic review of techniques. Health  Technol Assess. 2001;5(5):1-186.
  19. Straus SG Technology, Group Process, and Group Outcomes: Testing the Connections in Computer-Mediated and Face-to-Face Groups Human-Computer Interaction, Vol. 12, No. 3: pages 227-266. 1997.
  20. Van de Ven AH, Delbecq A.   The Effectiveness of Nominal, Delphi, and Interacting Group Decision  Making Process.  - Academy of Management Journal, 1974. 
  21. Vennix JAM. Group model-building:  tackling messy problems System Dynamics Review Volume 15, Issue 4, pages 379 - 401, 2000.
  22. Woudenberg F, An Evaluation of Delphi  - Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 1991

 

 
Copyright © 1996  Farrokh Alemi, Ph.D. Created on Saturday, September 21, 1996. Most recent revision done on 10/19/2017.  This page is part of the course on Decision Analysis, the lecture on Modeling Group's Decision.